Saturday, October 20, 2007

What does 'memorize' mean?

Last night I had a conversation with some faculty friends about teaching. We reached the common point where some of us were saying that students shouldn't have to memorize lists of facts, and others were saying that students need to know the facts before they can begin to think about what they mean. One of us made the important point that the word 'memorize' may mean different things to different people, and different things in different contexts, which (slowly) got me thinking about how we can be more clear.

We all want our students to remember the facts we think important. When we complain that students are memorizing rather than learning (student readers of this blog should note that we are just as likely to be blaming the teachers as the students for this), we mean to distinguish 'rote memorization' from our more-or-less vague concept of 'real learning'. Maybe we could speak of 'remembering without understanding' and remembering with understanding'.

I'll use the cell-division process of meiosis as an example. Students are often expected to be able to define meiosis, name the stages of meiosis and reproduce the textbook illustrations of these stages. But students can accomplish this by rote memorization or as part of a richer remembering. A student who 'really understood' meiosis might be able to explain how the consequences of meiosis differ from those of mitosis and what role this difference plays in reproduction. They might be able to draw steps intermediate between the defined stages, or move paper chromosomes to simulate the entire process. They might be able to explain the physical forces and interactions that bring about the different stages, and how the genetics principles called 'Mendel's rules' are a consequence of what happens to chromosomes in meiosis.

I'll try another example, brought up by a botanist who teaches students about how the different parts of plants transport water and nutrients. Students could simply rote-memorize the names of the structures (phloem, xylem, cambium, stomata, root hairs...) and be able to reproduce textbook definitions and drawings of them, complete with labels of the substances transported and the directions of flow. Or they could also be able to explain why plants need root hairs, why some substances move up the phloem and others down the xylem (or vice versa?), which parts of this transport consume energy and why other parts don't.

Of course a student could have used rote memorization to remember all this information. And we often test students' learning in ways that can be satisfied by rote memorization, probably because this is much easier for us to assess than is deeper understanding. Our Physics colleagues have been discovering that tests that they thought were assessing understanding were in fact being passed by rote memorization. Students could 'plug and chug' - getting the answer to a question of a recognized type by inserting numbers into a memorized formula. When physicists began to assess students' understanding by putting the phenomena into new (simpler and more familiar) contexts where formulas weren't useful, they discovered that the students could no longer answer the questions. So now Physics faculty are leading the way in devising ways to measure genuine understanding, and using these measures to identify and change weaknesses in their teaching.

In Biology we of course do try to test understanding, not just memorization. We do this by asking such questions as "Would anything go wrong if a cell started meiosis with three copies of one of its chromosomes?" or "Would a plant growing in a greenhouse on Mars need the same number of root hairs as one growing under identical conditions (light, water, nutrients, atmosphere) on Earth?" One reason that I give only open-book exams is to discourage myself from asking questions whose answers can simply be looked up in the book.

One big question for Biology faculty is whether our students should be asked to rote-memorize some information before they develop their understanding of its importance, or whether the remembering should only be built up (and assessed) as part of the understanding. I favour the latter. I have been thinking that some of my colleagues disagree, but this may be only because we've meant different things by the word 'memorize'.

3 comments:

Anonymous said...

Rosie, I really liked your blog entry. I think it gets at the heart of what we should be striving for (remembering with understanding) and also explains why we sometimes talk past each other. It's hard to imagine anybody arguing for "remembering without understanding". As a person who values the need for memorizing in biology (the beauty of which is like a tapestry with fascinating interwoven details), the anti-memorizing movement sounds a bit like an anti-knowing movement. But that's because, for me, memorizing is a process that involves familiarization and understanding. This, unfortunately, isn't always true for our students, and the key to moving forward is to help them distinguish the two types of remembering.

Anonymous said...

This blog is a splendid idea, thank you Rosie.

I wonder if the argument over learning is a bit too utilitarian. We are asking 'how' the students should be learning when we should be talking about why the students are learning (from their point of view) and through this we can get at what motivates or de-motivates a student.

In my experience "memorization" predominates when class sizes are large, facts are densely packed into class time with little discussion, and (most importantly) testing style is erratic and episodic-fact, rather than story (association and creativity), based.

But there are many permutations to this issue. You can't simply change testing style without also changing the mode of teaching. I have had some of my worst educational experiences at the hands of well meaning professors who taught in an episodic manner, then asked questions requiring synthesis and creativity, but hinged on (what I thought to be) fringe concepts. Miss the concept, fail the test, flip back to memorizing everything.

I think here, as elsewhere, the best intentions can result in the most damaging results (testing who got lucky and remembered fact A, or who didn't trust you to present the material and memorized everything).

On the other hand, testing of surface learning is standard. Both the student and the professor know where they stand. There is generally enough 'test fodder' that missing one concept will still allow for a reasonable outcome somewhere along the bell curve that's (often) nicely proportional to time spent studying.

I think we should dispose of this process altogether and switch to mentoring one on one of students (an idealistic dream, I know). I say this mainly because my very best educational experiences have always been one on one interactions, and the very worst have been in classrooms.

Unknown said...

I was so happy to see my most loved bloggers back to posting this awriter.org week. Don't hesitate to share some you could call your own most loved like in the post.